- Review
- Open access
- Published:
Event-related potential (ERP) markers of 22q11.2 deletion syndrome and associated psychosis
Journal of Neurodevelopmental Disorders volume 15, Article number: 19 (2023)
Abstract
22q11.2 deletion syndrome (22q11.2DS) is a multisystemic disorder characterized by a wide range of clinical features, ranging from life-threatening to less severe conditions. One-third of individuals with the deletion live with mild to moderate intellectual disability; approximately 60% meet criteria for at least one psychiatric condition.
22q11.2DS has become an important model for several medical, developmental, and psychiatric disorders. We have been particularly interested in understanding the risk for psychosis in this population: Approximately 30% of the individuals with the deletion go on to develop schizophrenia. The characterization of cognitive and neural differences between those individuals who develop schizophrenia and those who do not, despite being at genetic risk, holds important promise in what pertains to the clarification of paths to disease and to the development of tools for early identification and intervention.
Here, we review our previous event-related potential (ERP) findings as potential markers for 22q11.2DS and the associated risk for psychosis, while discussing others’ work. We focus on auditory processing (auditory-evoked potentials, auditory adaptation, and auditory sensory memory), visual processing (visual-evoked potentials and visual adaptation), and inhibition and error monitoring.
The findings discussed suggest basic mechanistic and disease process effects on neural processing in 22q11.2DS that are present in both early sensory and later cognitive processing, with possible implications for phenotype. In early sensory processes, both during auditory and visual processing, two mechanisms that impact neural responses in opposite ways seem to coexist—one related to the deletion, which increases brain responses; another linked to psychosis, decreasing neural activity. Later, higher-order cognitive processes may be equally relevant as markers for psychosis. More specifically, we argue that components related to error monitoring may hold particular promise in the study of risk for schizophrenia in the general population.
Background
22q11.2 deletion syndrome (22q11.2DS), also identified as velo-cardio-facial or DiGeorge syndrome, occurs in approximately 1:1524 to 1:4000 live births [1, 2]. This mostly de novo deletion results from meiotic recombination events in four regions known as A-D low-copy repeats (LCR) on the long (q) arm of chromosome 22. About 85% of those living with 22q11.2DS present a deletion of the entire 2.5- to 3-Mb LCR A-D region; the remainder have smaller nested deletions within that region [3, 4]. There are 90 known or predicted genes present in the 3-Mb 22q11.2 locus that are hemizygously deleted [5]. Of those, about 90% have documented expression in the brain and may affect early neuronal migration and cortical development [5,6,7].
22q112.DS is a multisystemic disorder characterized by a wide range of clinical features, ranging from life-threatening to less severe conditions [8]. Common medical issues involve congenital heart defects, palatal abnormalities, immunodeficiency, hypocalcemia, genitourinary defects, and feeding and gastrointestinal problems [9, 10]. Cognitively, the majority of individuals with 22q11.2DS present an intellectual level in the borderline range (IQ scores between 70 and 84), and about one-third live with mild to moderate intellectual disability [11]. Intellectual ability appears to relate, in this population, to deletion size: Individuals with smaller (A-B) deletions have modestly higher IQ scores than those with larger (A-D) deletions [12]. Although the neurocognitive profile associated with the syndrome is quite variable between individuals, 22q11.2DS is characterized by overall deficits in executive function [13,14,15,16], nonverbal memory [17, 18], visuospatial [19,20,21] and visual-motor [22] processing, and working memory [23]. Individuals living with 22q11.2DS are also at an increased risk for developing psychiatric conditions: Approximately 60% meet criteria for at least one psychiatric diagnosis. Attention deficit with hyperactivity disorder (ADHD), autism spectrum disorder (ASD), anxiety and mood disorders, and psychotic disorders and schizophrenia have all been described in association with the syndrome [24,25,26,27,28,29,30]. Increased risk for psychiatric conditions may be related to specific cognitive trajectories in 22q11.2DS. While different trajectories have been identified in the syndrome—of relatively stable IQ, decline as a result of stagnation of cognitive development relative to increasing cognitive requirements, and absolute loss of cognitive abilities [31, 32]—the latter appears to be particularly associated with an increased risk for developing a psychiatric disorder [33].
In the past decade, 22q11.2DS has become an important model for several medical, developmental, and psychiatric disorders. Allowing for a better understanding of different conditions, the study of 22q11.2DS provides unique opportunities to clarify trajectories from risk to expression of disease. Consequently, the potential for the development of translational strategies and early interventions increases for both individuals with 22q11.2DS and those with associated features in the general population.
We have been particularly interested in understanding the risk for psychosis in this population. With 20 to 40% of individuals identified with the deletion going on to receive a formal diagnosis of schizophrenia [27,28,29,30], such risk is one of the most significant concerns for parents of children with 22q11.2DS. Importantly, considering the overwhelming burden of severe mental illnesses such as schizophrenia, clarifying paths to disease and developing tools for early identification prior to frank disease onset hold real promise for those living with this type of condition. Indeed, the detection of neural vulnerability prior to observable symptoms is crucial for the development of interventions focused on prevention rather than on treatment. Of note, idiopathic and 22q11.2DS-associated schizophrenia present similar clinical paths [33, 34] and clinical presentations [35] and high concordance of neuroanatomic correlates [36,37,38,39,40,41], which suggests that comparable neural changes could be explaining psychotic symptomatology in both populations. With approximately half of the adolescents with 22q11.2DS showing schizotypical traits and experiencing transient psychotic states [42], subthreshold psychotic symptoms appear, however, to present earlier in this group when compared to individuals in the general population who develop schizophrenia.
Given that all individuals with 22q11.2DS are at genetic risk for schizophrenia but not all develop the illness, a promising approach to understand markers of risk and disease is the comparison between those who develop psychotic symptoms and those who do not, despite being at risk. Our work suggests differences in behavior and brain responses between these 22q11.2DS subgroups. For instance, while individuals with the deletion but no psychotic symptoms were as fast as their age-matched control peers while pressing a mouse button during a go/no-go task, those with the deletion and psychotic symptoms were significantly slower [43]. Additionally, increased early evoked responses during auditory and visual tasks are observed in individuals with 22q11.2DS without psychotic symptoms, whereas those with the deletion and psychotic symptoms show reduced responses, quite similar to findings in individuals living with schizophrenia [44, 45].
Here, supported by findings from our work and contextualized by the extant literature, we consider the promise of event-related potential (ERP) biomarkers of risk for psychosis in 22q11.2DS, with the ultimate goal of generating new and critical questions regarding not only the true utility of such markers, but also the biology underlying risk of psychosis. Biomarkers, characteristics that are measured as indicators of typical or atypical biological processes or responses to an exposure or intervention [46], should be highly reproduceable, accessible measures with a sizeable signal-to-noise ratio that are modified in dynamic and reliable ways as the clinical condition progresses [47]. Electroencephalography (EEG), an easily deployed non-invasive method that provides information at the millisecond scale, allows one to probe the spatiotemporal dynamics of information processing in the brain. Because of its high temporal resolution, it permits the distinction between early sensory, sensory-perceptual, and later cognitive stages of processing [48, 50] and thus allows one to determine the stage at which information processing may be impaired. ERPs, the scalp recorded voltage fluctuations of the EEG signal that are time locked to a particular event of interest are seen as a set of positive and negative deflections in the evoked response. These evoked responses, or components, reflect activity within and across an often complex network of cortical regions [51, 52].
Considering the focus of ours and others’ work, this review covers (1) auditory processing (auditory-evoked potentials, auditory adaptation, and auditory sensory memory), (2) visual processing (visual-evoked potentials and visual adaptation), and (3) inhibition and error monitoring. Importantly, these processes have been comprehensively investigated in schizophrenia and have been discussed as potential endophenotypes for the condition.
Auditory processing
Auditory-evoked potentials
Auditory-evoked potentials (AEPs) are electrical brain responses that follow the presentation of an auditory stimulus. AEPs can be subdivided into three sequences of waves, reflecting activity at different levels of processing: (1) brain stem response, occurring within the initial 8–12 ms; (2) middle-latency sequence, resulting from activity in thalamic nuclei and neurons in the primary auditory cortex and occurring between 8 and 50 ms; (3) long-latency or cortical responses, which reflect activity in higher-order auditory and association cortices and generally occur between 50 and 300 ms [52]. Here, the focus is on the latter and, more specifically, on components indexing basic auditory processing and auditory sensory memory.
Basic auditory processing
The auditory N1 is the first prominent negative AEP [50] and reflects neural activity generated in and around the primary auditory cortex [53]. In schizophrenia, N1 amplitudes are generally reduced [51, 54,55,56,57]. In contrast, increased N1 amplitudes appear to be observed in individuals with 22q11.2DS [58,59,60] and have been described in a 22q11.2DS mouse model [61]. Larger N1s have been associated with elevated activity in the anterior cingulate and dorsomedial frontal cortex [60] and associated with alterations in the cortical glutamate N-methyl-D-aspartate (NMDA) receptors [58, 62, 63] (see Fig. 1 for a simplified representation of the association between different neurotransmitters and brain mechanisms/processes addressed in the present review). Importantly, increased sensitivity to NMDA receptor antagonism has been described in a mouse model of 22q11.2DS [61] and elevated NMDA-receptor antibodies were found in a 19-year-old with the deletion and a history of cognitive decline and psychotic symptomatology [64].
Utilizing an oddball paradigm, we investigated basic auditory processing in individuals with 22q11.2DS. Unique to other studies on this topic [58, 59, 65,66,67,68], we focused on the potential differences between those with and without psychotic symptoms. We showed that while those without psychotic symptoms recapitulate the abovementioned increased N1 amplitudes, individuals with one or more psychotic symptoms showed reduced N1s (Fig. 2A), as is typically described in schizophrenia [51, 54,55,56,57,58,59,60,61,62,63,64,65,66,67,68,69]. Such reductions have been argued to index genetic risk for psychosis, as they have been described in first-degree relatives of individuals living with schizophrenia [57]. Our work, however, suggests otherwise, as those with the deletion and no psychotic symptoms (but still at-risk for psychosis) presented, here, increased amplitudes. Of note, these group differences were most evident in the differential response to different stimulation rates and appeared to be largely driven by longer stimulus onset asynchronies (SOA) conditions (900 and 1800 ms versus 450 ms) (see Fig. 2A), as discussed in the next section (Auditory Adaptation). We believe that there may be two mechanisms at work during early auditory sensory processing in 22q11.2DS: One related to the deletion resulting in the increased amplitudes observed in human and non-human animals; another associated with the presence of psychotic symptoms, which has as its outcome a decrease in brain responses. The auditory N1 may be modulated by both a deletion in chromosome 22 and the presence of psychotic symptomatology.
A Averaged ERPs (standard tones) per group (CT 22q, 22q − (without psychotic symptoms), 22q + (with psychotic symptoms)) and SOA (450, 900, 1800) at the fronto-central channel FCz. B Difference waves (deviants-standards) per group (CT 22q, 22q − , 22q +) and SOA (450, 900, 1800) at the fronto-central channel FCz. Adapted from [44]
Auditory adaptation
Sensory adaptation is an important property of sensory processing, as it appears to attenuate system redundancy [70,71,72,73]. Typically, in faster versus slower presentation rates of stimuli, amplitude reductions are observed, which can be explained by temporal limitations intrinsic to mechanistic brain response generation. That is, faster presentations do not allow for full response recovery, which results in decreased amplitudes [50, 74,75,76,77,78]. Other explanations of adaptation, related to priming and/or expectation have also been considered [79]. Though the nature of the neural mechanisms underlying adaptation is not fully understood, mechanisms such as neuronal refractoriness and presynaptic calcium influx have been implicated, as have NMDA-mediated glutamate transmission and GABAergic inhibition [80,81,82,83] (see Fig. 1).
In schizophrenia, altered adaptation to repetitive auditory stimuli has been reported [84,85,86,87], but this phenomenon has been sparsely studied in 22q11.2DS. The few studies published do not, moreover, provide a clear picture. While using sensory gating paradigms, some show intact [60, 88], others impaired [68] P50s. The P50 sensory gating is a measure of the suppression of the second P50 relative to the first P50 ERP in a paired-click paradigm assumed to reflect the ability to filter out repetitive, irrelevant stimuli and thus minimize information overload [89]. One study employing an oddball paradigm found reduced intrinsic connection within the right primary auditory cortex in a group of individuals with 22q112.DS but no psychotic symptoms. Such reduction was interpreted as suggestive of decreased adaptation in 22q11.2DS [59].
Our data, derived from a duration oddball paradigm with three different blocked SOAs (450, 900, and 1800 ms) supports “typical” adaptation in the N1 time window in 22q11.2DS, as can be appreciated in Fig. 2A. Indeed, regardless of the presence of psychotic symptoms, individuals with the deletion showed adaptation effects [44]. A distinction between those with and those without psychotic symptoms revealed, however, that, while the 22q11.2DS sub-group without psychotic symptoms showed increased adaptation effects, individuals with one or more symptoms showed decreased effects, compared to a control group. Interestingly, a closer look at our data revealed that it was the responses at the slower presentation rates that differed in the 22q11.2DS group, being larger in the 22q11.2DS group overall and in the sub-group without symptoms (Fig. 2A). These data seem to reiterate the presence of two different but important mechanisms in this early time window of sensory processing: One mechanism relating to 22q11.2DS and resulting in amplitude and adaptation effects increases, the other related to psychosis and causing overall amplitude reductions.
Auditory sensory memory
Auditory sensory memory can be defined as a preattentive memory system that allows an individual to retain traces of sensory information after the termination of the original stimulus [90]. This very short memory can be assessed by means of the so-called mismatch negativity (MMN), generated during oddball paradigms. The MMN occurs when a repeating stimulus (the standard) in a sequence is replaced by a deviant stimulus. By violating the memory trace formed by the regularity of the consecutively presented standards, the deviant induces an MMN [91]. Typically ocurring 100 to 200 ms after the deviant, the MMN is thought to reflect largely preattentive neural processes underlying detection of a pattern violation and updating of a representation of a regularity in the auditory environment [92,93,94]. This component appears to be primarily mediated by the NMDA receptor [95] (Fig. 1).
Reduced MMN amplitudes have consistently been shown in schizophrenia (for reviews, see [96, 97]) in at-risk [98,99,100,101,102], recent onset [99, 100, 103,104,105], and chronic [99, 103, 106,107,108,109,110,111,112,113] stages of the condition, although findings are not always consistent and this is especially the case in first-episode and at-risk individuals where a number of negative findings have been reported [111, 114,115,116,117]. In 22q11.2DS, evidence is even less consistent (see, for a review, [118]). Whereas reduced pitch and duration [65] and frequency MMNs [58] have been reported, others have failed to show differences between individuals with 22q11.2DS and their control peers in frequency [59, 68], intensity, directionally, and duration deviants [68].
Such inconsistencies may again be a function of the phenotypic heterogeneity that is characteristic of 22q11.2DS. We had thus hypothesized that, as observed in the N1 time window, differences in MMN amplitudes would be found between those with and those without psychotic symptoms. It was therefore with surprise that we not only failed to observe differences between those two 22q11.2DS sub-groups, but we actually showed a slightly enhanced MMN in this clinical population [44] (see Fig. 2B). Given the well-established MMN reductions in schizophrenia and the weaker memory traces described in 22q11.2DS (see [119]), these findings were unexpected. That only a few of the individuals with 22q112.DS tested in this set of studies had a diagnosis of schizophrenia (the majority presented with subthreshold psychosis), and that our 22q11.2DS participants were younger than the typical individual with schizophrenia and have thus been living with symptoms for a significantly shorter period of time, likely explains the lack of effects seen in the MMN. Considering the inconsistent findings regarding MMN amplitudes in 22q11.2DS, the potential of this component as a biomarker of psychosis risk is not clear in this population.
Visual processing
Visual-evoked potentials
Visual-evoked potentials (VEPs) refer to electrical potentials recorded from the occipital and parietal scalp over the visual cortex. Here, the focus is on components evoked within the first 200 ms after stimulation.
Basic visual processing
Early visual-evoked responses are generally reduced in schizophrenia (e.g., [120,121,122,123,124,125,126,127,128,129,130,131]). In 22q11.2DS, a combination of amplitude decreases and increases has been reported in response to visual stimulation [132, 133]. While reductions have been observed in P1 and N1 amplitudes, increased global amplitudes seem to emerge in time windows later than those traditionally associated with sensory-perceptual processing, around 250 ms [132]. These later increases in (frontal) activity in 22q11.2DS could reflect an increased recruitment of frontal regions to compensate for reduced activity in earlier processing in visual cortex [132]. Consistent with these EEG findings, there is evidence of atypical development and connectivity of occipital brain regions in this population. In a magnetic resonance imaging (MRI) study, widespread loss of white matter extending bilaterally in (among others) occipito-parietal regions was found in a small sample of adults with the deletion [134]. Given that glutamate is a crucial player in the neurotransmission within visual pathways, the proline dehydrogenase (PRODH) gene, a gene whose haploinsufficiency contributes to the clinical phenotype of 22q11.2DS, has been argued as a possible susceptibility gene for visual processing differences in this clinical population [133] (Fig. 1).
Extending our investigation of early sensory processes as potential markers in 22q11.2DS and associated psychosis to visual processing, we utilized a visual adaptation paradigm previously used by our research group [120, 135] to question possible neural differences between those with and without psychotic symptoms. Much like what we found for auditory processing [44], we showed increased ERP amplitudes at around 100 ms for the group without psychotic symptoms, compared to those with psychotic symptoms and to age-matched controls [45] (Fig. 3A). Hence, the thesis of enhanced sensory processing in 22q11.2DS when in the absence of psychotic symptomatology appears to be likewise applicable to early visual processing. The groups also differed in a later (~ 200 ms) stage of processing, with those with the deletion and psychotic symptoms showing reduced amplitudes when compared to those without. Accordingly, while increased amplitudes in the earlier time window may reflect specific neurogenetic aspects associated with a deletion in chromosome 22, reduced amplitudes in the later window may be a marker of the presence and/or chronicity/severity of psychosis.
A Averaged ERPs per group (CT 22q, 22q − (without psychotic symptoms), 22q + (with psychotic symptoms), and SZ) at the occipital channel Oz, showing the average of all ISIs. B Curves representing visual adaptation effects between groups (CT 22q, 22q − , 22q + , and SZ) between 165 and 205 ms. Error bars represent standard errors of the mean. Adapted from [45]
Alterations in NMDA receptors have been associated with increased early and decreased later neural responses [58, 62, 63, 136]. NMDA-related dysfunctional mechanisms may impact the modulation of sensory information reflected by early components and the efficiency of early attentional processes indexed by later components [136]. Atypical synaptic plasticity in early visual processing areas, resulting from both altered glutamate and dopamine levels [137, 138]—modulated by PRODH during early brain development [139]—could explain differences in visual sensory processing in 22q11.2DS (Fig. 1).
Visual adaptation
Sensory adaptation is an important property of sensory processing [70,71,72,73], as indicated in the Auditory Adaptation section above. In schizophrenia, reduced visual adaption has also been reported [120, 140, 141], but see [84], though less consistently than in the auditory domain. To the best of our knowledge, no published study other than ours has addressed visual adaption in 22q11.2DS.
Utilizing a simple checkerboard adaptation task with five different interstimulus intervals (ISIs; 145, 245, 495, 995, and 2495 ms), we showed increased visual adaptation effects in individuals with the deletion, particularly in those without psychotic symptoms [45] (Fig. 3B). While larger adaptation effects could reflect better visual encoding efficiency and relate to enhanced visual short-term memory and attentional processes [142,143,144, 144], such a possibility seems exceedingly unlikely in 22q11.2DS, in which difficulties have been noted in visuospatial memory, attention, working memory, and other executive-type functions [17, 19, 44, 145,146,147]. As the neural mechanisms underlying adaptation are not well understood, the mechanistic implications of enlarged adaptation in 22q11.2DS remain relatively elusive. Interestingly, those with psychotic symptoms presented reduced adaptation effects, as expected in schizophrenia. In schizophrenia, reduced visual adaptation has been argued to contribute to the visual perception differences characteristic in psychosis [148]. Given that one proposed function of adaptation is that it improves discriminability of novel stimuli [149] and that one explanation for psychotic symptoms is the inappropriate attribution of salience to irrelevant stimuli, this hypothesis is particularly relevant for schizophrenia [150] and for those with 22q11.2DS and psychotic symptoms. These findings affirm, once again, that there are likely two processes at play here, one being the neurogenetic influence of a deletion in chromosome 22, and the other being the impact of psychosis on sensory processing. The similarity across modalities is, moreover, striking. Basic auditory and visual processing and adaptation fall into the basic and largely obligatory sensory processing domain. While they can be influenced by a participant’s state of alertness and by the presence or absence of directed attention, these influences are relatively small. Consequently, they are particularly appealing as candidate biomarkers.
Inhibition and error monitoring
Inhibition and error monitoring are higher-order processes that fall in the realm of executive functioning. Briefly, executive function is an umbrella term that groups the set of cognitive control processes that govern goal-directed behavior and serve to optimize performance on complex tasks, allowing one to be flexible and to adapt to novel, ever-changing circumstances [151]. As these processes are crucial for function and interventional strategies can target their remediation, work on executive function components carries true potential for cognitive and daily function improvement.
Inhibitory control
Inhibitory control, the process by which one suppresses a prepotent response that is irrelevant or inappropriate in a particular context, is essential for adjusting behavior dynamically with changing environmental contexts [152,153,154]. In schizophrenia, there is ample behavioral and EEG evidence of differences in inhibitory processes [155,156,157,158,159,160] and this has also been found in 22q11.2DS [161, 162]. Using a Go/No-Go EEG task (as in [161, 162], we asked whether neural differences related to inhibitory control in individuals with 22q11.2DS were modulated by the presence of psychotic symptoms. Our analyses focused on ERP components that are typically evoked during similar Go/No-Go tasks: The No-Go N2, a negative-going ERP component peaking between 200 and 300 ms and representing early, automatic inhibitory [163,164,165,166] and/or conflict detection processes [167,168,169], and the No-Go P3, a positive potential that peaks at about 300–500 ms, argued as a marker of response inhibition [170,171,172,173,174], stimulus evaluation [175,176,177] and adaptive, more effortful forms of control [165, 166, 178].
As can be appreciated in Fig. 4A, our data revealed no differences between 22q11.2DS and controls or between those with and those without psychotic symptoms in the No-Go N2, possibly reflecting the lack of a clear N2 effect in either group. The N2 has been argued as a less reliable marker of response inhibition than the P3 [179,180,181]. Reductions in the No-Go P3 were, however, observed in 22q11.2DS, confirming possible difficulties in inhibitory-related processes (Fig. 4A). Additionally, those with smaller P3s performed worse in a standardized inhibition task, arguing for the No-Go P3 as reflecting the ability to inhibit a prepotent response. Interestingly, the No-Go P3 was only reduced in the 22q11.2DS group with psychotic symptoms, suggesting that this component could be a potential marker of the presence of psychotic symptoms [43] (Fig. 4A). Indeed, P3 reductions in schizophrenia [182] have been associated with disease severity, regardless of medication intake and task demands [183]. Amplitude reductions in this time window have, however, been shown in many other conditions such as chronic alcoholism [184] and ADHD [185], and thus, rather than conceiving a reduced P3 as a signature of schizophrenia-specific processes, one should probably conceptualize it as indexing general cognitive impairment in conditions characterized by inhibition deficits. Still, P3 could be useful in differentiating, within the 22q11.2DS population, those at higher risk to develop schizophrenia. P3 may reflect the activity of the neuromodulatory locus coeruleus (LC)–norepinephrine (NE) producing nucleus [186]. Norepinephrine dysfunction has been described in schizophrenia [187] and seems to be associated with its characteristic cognitive deficits [188]. COMT, encoding the protein catechol-O-methyltransferase responsible for degrading catecholamines such as norepinephrine (particularly in the prefrontal cortex), is a gene in the 22q11.2 region and is often thought of as a risk gene candidate for psychiatric disorders [8] (Fig. 1).
A No-Go P3 (difference waves: correct rejections – hits) per group (CT 22q, 22q − (without psychotic symptoms), 22q + (with psychotic symptoms), SZ) at the centro-parietal channel CPz. B Averaged ERPs depicting error-related positivity (Pe) per group at CPz. Adapted from [43]
Error monitoring
Error monitoring involves identification and correction of deviance from a correct response [189], and is required to achieve goal-directed behavior, to make appropriate adjustments to behavior, and to maintain task performance [190]. Two ERP components are often associated with error monitoring: The error-related negativity (ERN or Ne), a component occurring within 100 ms of an erroneous response, argued to reflect a mismatch between response selection and response execution [191, 192], but not remedial action [192]; and the error-related positivity (Pe), a component peaking between 200 and 500 ms post incorrect-response, which has been suggested to reflect conscious error processing or updating of error context [192, 193]. Reductions of Pe suggest a weakened (or even absent) sense of error awareness [194].
In schizophrenia, Ne is attenuated [195,196,197,198,199] and has been argued as a potentially important marker of risk, as such reductions have been shown in children with antecedents of schizophrenia [200], in high-risk individuals, early in the course of the disease, and in those living with chronic schizophrenia [201]. Though less consistently so, Pe reductions have likewise been reported in schizophrenia [201, 202], but see [197, 199, 203, 204]. To our knowledge, other than ours, no other study investigating these components in 22q11.2DS had been published previous to this review.
To investigate the potential of error monitoring-related components as markers of 22q11.2DS and/or the associated risk for psychosis, we focused on the neural activity following false alarms (i.e., responses to non-targets) in a Go/No-Go task [43] and compared individuals with 22q11.2DS with controls and individuals living with schizophrenia. As can be seen in Fig. 4B, for both Ne and Pe, our data revealed that not only did 22q11.2DS differ from controls, showing significantly reduced amplitudes, but no differences were seen between the two 22q11.2DS sub-groups. Ne and Pe may be potential markers of risk for schizophrenia, as all individuals with 22q11.2DS, regardless of the presence of psychotic symptoms, showed virtually absent componentry following the errors committed, a pattern that was no different to that which we observed in a group of individuals with chronic schizophrenia.
Conclusion
Focusing on our previous work on auditory and visual sensory and higher-order cognitive (inhibition and error monitoring) processes in 22q11.DS, we reviewed and discussed those findings in the context of defining potential markers in this clinical population, particularly pertaining to risk for psychosis. As we focus on the mostly uncharted comparison between individuals with 22q11.2DS with and without psychotic symptoms and argue for what we believe is the potential impact of such comparison, not only for those living with the syndrome, but also for other at-risk groups, we believe that this review could generate new and critical questions with potential to advance the definition of meaningful markers of risk for psychosis. Figure 5 summarizes the findings discussed here.
Graphic summary of findings, per process discussed and per group (22q11.2DS without psychotic symptoms: 22q − , 22q11.2DS with psychotic symptoms: 22q + , schizophrenia: SZ). Green, upward-pointing arrows represent increased response; Pink, downward pointing arrows represent decreased response. * Findings for basic auditory processing and auditory adaptation are based on the literature
Some work has been done in the past decade that approaches 22q11.2DS as a model for schizophrenia. Most studies have not, however, made a clear distinction or comparison between those individuals with the deletion and psychotic symptoms and those without psychotic symptomatology. The data discussed here clearly stress the importance of distinguishing between those with 22q11.2DS and psychotic symptoms and those without in the search for biomarkers. Indeed, in both auditory and visual sensory processing and executive function-related processes, we describe significant differences between those two sub-groups. In our opinion, it is in these differences that true understanding of pathways to disease may rest. Importantly, this approach can be generalized to any other associated features for which clear sub-groups can be formed. In characterizing phenotypically heterogenous syndromes such as 22q11.2DS, though it remains important to understand overall function, it is in the consideration of individual differences and sub-group trajectories that tangible potential for translational and interventional strategies may be found. For instance, it may be critical to consider individual differences in cognitive functioning and the extent to which those do or do not account for some of the findings reported in this review (of note, the 22q − and 22q + groups compared here did not differ in IQ scores). As argued for in schizophrenia [205], specific components or measures of cognitive function (e.g., verbal and non-verbal abilities) may serve as protective mechanisms in 22q11.2DS and should thus be leveraged.
The findings discussed here suggest basic mechanistic and disease process effects on neural processing in 22q11.2DS that are evident in early sensory and later cognitive processing, with possible implications for phenotype. As discussed in each section, most of the findings reported may be explained by processes related to the NMDA receptor complex and glutamatergic and GABAergic modulations, all possibly associated with PRODH and COMT, two of the genes in the region 11.2 in the long arm of chromosome 22 (see Fig. 1). The thorough investigation of NMDA-related function in 22q11.2DS, given its described associations with different components of sensory processing and seminal evidence of dysfunction in humans with the syndrome, animal models of 22q11.2DS, and in individuals living with schizophrenia, may hold particular promise in further characterizing the biology of (risk for) psychosis.
In summary, in early sensory processes, both during auditory and visual processing, two mechanisms that impact neural responses in opposite ways seem to coexist—one related to the deletion, which increases brain responses; another linked to psychosis, which decreases neural activity. Whether the amplitude enhancement in individuals with 22q11.2DS and no psychotic symptoms serves as a protective mechanism or is a mere consequence of the deletion, remains elusive. Additionally, it is unclear whether the decreased neural activity observed in those with psychotic symptoms is secondary to psychotic features or inherent to those who will develop them. Clarifying this matter is of incredible value to those living with a chromosome 22q11.2 deletion, but also to individuals in the general population who are at risk for developing schizophrenia. Indeed, it remains crucial to differentiate the global neural effects of living with psychosis from the biology contributing to psychosis itself. Longitudinal studies allowing for the study of the prodromal phase of psychosis could hold particular potential here.
We further show that later, higher-order cognitive processes may be equally relevant as markers for psychosis. More specifically, we argue that components related to error monitoring (Ne and Pe) may hold promise in the study of risk for schizophrenia in the general population. A more thorough, longitudinal investigation of these potential markers, the roles of genes in the 22q11.2 region in the conversion to psychosis, and NMDA-related dysfunctional mechanisms has potential to advance our knowledge about the contribution of specific neural and genetic processes (and of their interactions) to the onset of schizophrenia. Though the focus of the present manuscript is on the association between psychosis and a chromosome 22q11.2 deletion, other neurodevelopmental disorders have been observed in individuals living with the deletion. Future research should investigate if the presence of conditions such as ASD and ADHD, or symptoms related to these diagnoses, affect the pattern of findings reported here and take the complex inter-influences of clinical and cognitive variables into consideration.
Availability of data and materials
The datasets referred to in this manuscript are available from the corresponding authors on reasonable request. Analysis pipelines can be accessed at github.com/DouweHorsthuis.
References
Jacobsson B, Clifton R, Egbert M, Malone F, Wapner RJ, Roman AS, et al. Cell-free DNA screening for prenatal detection of 22q11. 2 deletion syndrome. Am J Obstet Gynecol. 2022;227(1):79-e1-.e11.
McDonald-McGinn DM, Sullivan KE. Sullivan KE. Chromosome 22q11. 2 deletion syndrome (DiGeorge syndrome/velocardiofacial syndrome). Med. 2011;90(1):1–18.
Edelmann L, Pandita RK, Morrow BE. Low-copy repeats mediate the common 3-Mb deletion in patients with velo-cardio-facial syndrome. Am J Hum Genet. 1999;64(4):1076–86.
Morrow BE, McDonald‐McGinn DM, Emanuel BS, Vermeesch JR, Scambler PJ. Molecular genetics of 22q11. 2 deletion syndrome. Am J Med Genet A. 2018;176(10):2070–81.
Guna A, Butcher NJ, Bassett AS. Comparative mapping of the 22q11. 2 deletion region and the potential of simple model organisms. J Neurodev Disord. 2015;7(1):1–16.
Karayiorgou M, Simon TJ, Gogos JA. 22q11.2 microdeletions: Linking DNA structural variation to brain dysfunction and schizophrenia. Nat Rev Neurosci. 2010;11(6):402–16.
Meechan DW, Tucker ES, Maynard TM, LaMantia A. Diminished dosage of 22q11 genes disrupts neurogenesis and cortical development in a mouse model of 22q11 deletion/DiGeorge syndrome. Proc Natl Acad Sci. 2009;106(38):16434–45.
Philip N, Bassett AS. Cognitive, behavioural and psychiatric phenotype in 22q11.2 deletion syndrome. Behav Genet. 2011;41(3):403–12.
McDonald-McGinn DM, Sullivan KE, Marino B, Philip N, Swillen A, Vorstman JA, et al. 22q11.2 deletion syndrome. Nat Rev Dis Primers. 2015;1:15071.
Shprintzen RJ, Goldberg R, Golding-Kushner KJ, Marion RW. Late-onset psychosis in the velo-cardio-facial syndrome. Am J Med Genet. 1992;42:141–2.
Swillen A, McDonald‐McGinn D. McDonald‐McGinn D. Developmental trajectories in 22q11. 2 deletion syndrome. Am J Med Genet C Semin Med Genet. 2015;169C:172–81.
Zhao Y, Guo T, Fiksinski A, Breetvelt E, McDonald‐McGinn DM, Crowley TB, et al. Variance of IQ is partially dependent on deletion type among 1,427 22q11. 2 deletion syndrome subjects. Am J Med Genet A. 2018;176(10):2172–81.
Bish JP, Ferrante SM, McDonald-McGinn D, Zackai E, Simon TJ. Maladaptive conflict monitoring as evidence for executive dysfunction in children with chromosome 22q11.2 deletion syndrome. Dev Sci. 2005;8(1):36–43.
Rockers K, Ousley O, Sutton T, Schoenberg E, Coleman K, Walker E, et al. Performance on the modified card sorting test and its relation to psychopathology in adolescents and young adults with 22q11.2 deletion syndrome. J Intellect Disabil Res. 2009 53(7):665–76.
Sobin C, Kiley-Brabeck K, Daniels S, Blundell M, Anyane-Yeboa K, Karayiorgou M. Networks of attention in children with the 22q11 deletion syndrome. Dev Neuropsychol. 2004;26(2):611–26.
Van Aken K, Swillen A, Beirinckx M, Janssens L, Caeyenberghs K, Smits-Engelsman B. Prospective control abilities during visuo-manual tracking in children with 22q11.2 Deletion syndrome compared to age- and IQ-matched controls. Res Dev Disabil. 2010;31(3):634–41.
Bearden CE, Woodin MF, Wang PP, Moss E, McDonald-McGinn D, Zackai E, et al. The neurocognitive phenotype of the 22q11.2 deletion syndrome: selective deficit in visual-spatial memory. J Clin Exp Neuropsychol. 2001;23(4):447–64.
Swillen A, Devriendt K, Legius E, Prinzie P, Vogels A, Ghesquière P, et al. The behavioural phenotyoe in velo-cardio-facial syndrome (VCFS): From infancy to adolescence. Genetic Counceling. 1999;10(1):79–88.
Antshel KM, Fremont W, Kates WR. The neurocognitive phenotype in velo-cardio-facial syndrome: a developmental perspective. Dev Disabil Res Rev. 2008;14(1):43–51.
Cabaral M, Beaton EA, Stoddard J, Simon TJ. Impaired multiple object tracking in children with chromosome 22q11.2 deletion syndrome. J Neurodev Disord. 2012;4(6):1–8.
Simon TJ, Bish JP, Bearden CE, Ding L, Ferrante S, Nguyen V, et al. A multilevel analysis of cognitive dysfunction and psychopathology associated with chromossome 22q11.2 deletion syndrome in children. Dev Psychopathology. 2005;17(3):753–84.
Howley SA, Prasad SE, Pender NP, Murphy KC. Relationship between reaction time, fine motor control, and visual-spatial perception on vigilance and visual-motor tasks in 22q11.2 Deletion Syndrome. Res Dev Disabil. 2012;33(5):1495–502.
Lajiness-O’Neill RR, Beaulieu I, Titus JB, Asamoah A, Bigler ED, Bawle EV, et al. Memory and learning in children with 22q11.2 deletion syndrome: evidence for ventral and dorsal stream disruption? Child Neuropsychol. 2005;11(1):55–71.
Murphy KC. Schizophrenia and velo-cardio-facial syndrome. The Lancet. 2002;359(9304):426–30.
Bassett AS, Chow EWC, Weksberg R. Chromosomal abnormalities and schizophrenia. Am J Med Genet. 2000;97:45–51.
Baker K, Vorstman JA. Is there a core neuropsychiatric phenotype in 22q11.2 deletion syndrome? Curr Opin Neurol. 2012;25(2):131–7.
Bassett AS, Chow EWC. 22q11 deletion syndrome: A genetic subtype of schizophrenia. Biol Psychiat. 1999;10:882–91.
Monks S, Niarchou M, Davies AR, Walters JT, Williams N, Owen MJ, et al. Further evidence for high rates of schizophrenia in 22q11. 2 deletion syndrome. Schizophrenia Res. 2014;153(1–3):231–6.
Murphy KC, Jones LA, Owen MJ. High rates of schizophrenia in adults with velo-cardio-facial syndrome. Arch Gen Psychiatry. 1999;56:940–5.
Schneider M, Debbané M, Bassett AS, Chow EW, Fung WLA, van den Bree MB, et al. Psychiatric disorders from childhood to adulthood in 22q11. 2 deletion syndrome: results from the International Consortium on Brain and Behavior in 22q11. 2 Deletion Syndrome. Am J Psychiatry. 2014;171(6):627–39.
Duijff SN, Klaassen PW, de Veye HFS, Beemer FA, Sinnema G, Vorstman JA. Cognitive development in children with 22q11. 2 deletion syndrome. Br J Psychiatry. 2012;200(6):462–8.
Duijff SN, Klaassen PW, de Veye HFS, Beemer FA, Sinnema G, Vorstman JA. Cognitive and behavioral trajectories in 22q11DS from childhood into adolescence: A prospective 6-year follow-up study. Res Dev Disabil. 2013;34(9):2937–45.
Vorstman JA, Breetvelt E, Duijff SN, Eliez S, Schneider M, Jalbrzikowski M, et al. Cognitive decline preceding the onset of psychosis in patients with 22q11.2 deletion syndrome. JAMA Psychiatry. 2015;72(4):377–85.
Schneider M, Armando M, Pontillo M, Vicari S, Debbane M, Schultze-Lutter F, et al. Ultra high risk status and transition to psychosis in 22q11.2 deletion syndrome. World Psychiatry. 2016;15(3):259–65.
Tang SX, M MT, Calkins ME, Yi JJ, Savitt A, Kohler CG, et al. The Psychosis Spectrum in 22q11.2 Deletion Syndrome Is Comparable to That of Nondeleted Youths. Biol Psychiatry. 2017;82(17):25.
Kwon JS, Shenton ME, Hirayasu Y, Salisbury DF, Fischer IA, Dickey CC, et al. MRI study of cavum septi pellucidi in schizophrenia, affective disorder, and schizotypal personality disorder. Am J Psychiatry. 1998;155(4):509–15.
Nopoulos P, Swayze V, Flaum M, Ehrhardt JC, Yuh WTC, Andreasen NC. Cavum septi pellucidi in normals and patients with schizophrenia as detected by magnetic resonance imaging. Biol Psychiat. 1997;41:1102–8.
Bish JP, Pendyal A, Ding L, Ferrante H, Nguyen V, McDonald-McGinn D, et al. Specific cerebellar reductions in children with chromosome 22q11.2 deletion syndrome. Neurosci Lett. 2006;399(3):245–8.
Chow EWC, Mikulis DJ, Zipursky RB, Scutt LE, Weksberg R, Bassett AS. Qualitative MRI findings in adults with 22q11 deletion syndrome and schizophrenia. Biol Psychiat. 1999;46:1436–42.
Eliez S, Schmitt JE, White CD, Wellis VG, Riss AL. A Quantitative MRI study of posterior fossa development in velocardiofacial syndrome. Biol Psychiat. 2001;49:540–6.
Van Amelsvoort T, Daly E, Henry J, Robertson D, Ng V, Owen M, et al. Brain anatomy in adults with velocardiofacial syndrome with and without schizophrenia. Arch Gen Psychiatry. 2004;61:1085–96.
Baker K, Skuse DH. Adolescents and young adults with 22q11 deletion syndrome: Psychopathology in an at-risk group. Br J Psychiatry. 2005;186:115–20.
Francisco AA, Horsthuis DJ, Popiel M, Foxe JJ, Molholm S. Atypical response inhibition and error processing in 22q11. 2 Deletion Syndrome and schizophrenia: Towards neuromarkers of disease progression and risk. Neuroimage Clin. 2020;27:102351.
Francisco AA, Foxe JJ, Horsthuis DJ, DeMaio D, Molholm S. Assessing auditory processing endophenotypes associated with Schizophrenia in individuals with 22q11. 2 deletion syndrome. Transl Psychiatry. 2020;10(1):1–11.
Francisco AA, Foxe JJ, Horsthuis DJ, Molholm S. Early visual processing and adaptation as markers of disease, not vulnerability: EEG evidence from 22q11. 2 deletion syndrome, a population at high risk for schizophrenia. Schizophrenia. 2022;8(1):1–12.
Biomarker Working Group F-N. BEST (Biomarkers, Endpoints, and other Tools) Resource. In: Spring S, editor. BEST (Biomarkers, Endpoints, and other Tools) Resource. Silver Spring (MD): FDA-NIH; 2016.
García-Gutiérrez MS, Navarrete F, Sala F, Gasparyan A, Austrich-Olivares A, Manzanares J. Biomarkers in psychiatry: Concept, definition, types and relevance to the clinical reality. Frontiers in Psychiatry. 2020;11(432).
Foxe JJ, Simpson GV. Flow of activation from V1 to frontal cortex in humans. Exp Brain Res. 2002;142(1):139–50.
Lucan JN, Foxe JJ, Gomez-Ramirez M, Sathian K, Molholm S. Tactile shape discrimination recruits human lateral occipital complex during early perceptual processing. Hum Brain Mapp. 2010;31(11):1813–21.
Näätänen R, Picton T. The N1 wave of the human electric and magnetic response to sound: A review and an analysis of the component structure. Psychophysiology. 1987;24(4):375–425.
Leavitt VM, Molholm S, Ritter W, Shpaner M, Foxe JJ. Auditory processing in schizophrenia during the middle latency period (10–50 ms): high-density electrical mapping and source analysis reveal subcortical antecedents to early cortical deficits. J Psychiatry Neurosci. 2007;32(5):339–53.
Picton TW, Hillyard SA, Krausz HI, Galambos R. Human auditory evoked potentials. I: Evaluation of components. Electroencephalogr Clin Neurophysiol. 1974;36:179–90.
Giard MH, Perrin F, Echallier JF, Thevenet M, Froment JC, Pernier J. Dissociation of temporal and frontal components in the human auditory N1 wave: A scalp current density and dipole model analysis. Electroencephalogr Clin Neurophysiol. 1994;92:238–53.
Strik WK, Dierks T, Boning J, Osterheider M, Caspari A, Korber J. Disorders of smooth pursuit eye movement and auditory N100 in schizphrenic patients. Psychiatry Res. 1992;41:227–35.
Brockhaus-Dumke A, Schultze-Lutter F, Mueller R, Tendolkar I, Bechdolf A, Pukrop R, et al. Sensory gating in schizophrenia: P50 and N100 gating in antipsychotic-free subjects at risk, first-episode, and chronic patients. Biol Psychiat. 2008;64(5):376–84.
Salisbury DF, Collins KC, McCarley RW. Reductions in the N1 and P2 auditory event-related potentials in first-hospitalized and chronic schizophrenia. Schizophr Bull. 2010;36(5):991–1000.
Foxe JJ, Yeap S, Snyder AC, Kelly SP, Thakore JH, Molholm S. The N1 auditory evoked potential component as an endophenotype for schizophrenia: High-density electrical mapping in clinically unaffected first-degree relatives, first-episode, and chronic schizophrenia patients. Eur Arch Psychiatry Clin Neurosci. 2011;261(5):331–9.
Cantonas LM, Tomescu MI, Biria M, Jan RK, Schneider M, Eliez S, et al. Abnormal development of early auditory processing in 22q11.2 Deletion Syndrome. Transl Psychiatry. 2019;9(138):1–12.
Larsen KM, Morup M, Birknow MR, Fischer E, Hulme O, Vangkilde A. Altered auditory processing and effective connectivity in 22q11.2 deletion syndrome. Schizophrenia Res. 2018;197:328–36.
Rihs TA, Tomescu MI, Britz J, Rochas V, Custo A, Schneider M, et al. Altered auditory processing in frontal and left temporal cortex in 22q11.2 deletion syndrome: A group at high genetic risk for schizophrenia. Psychiatry Res. 2013;212(2):141–9.
Didriksen M, Fejgin K, Nilsson SRO, Birknow MR, Grayton HM, Larsen PH, et al. Persistent gating deficit and increased sensitivity to NMDA receptor antagonism after puberty in a new mouse model of the human 22q11.2 microdeletion syndrome: a study in male mice. J Psychiatry Neurosci. 2017;42(1):48–58.
Oranje B, van Berckel BNM, Kemner C, van Ree JM, Kahn RS, Verbaten MN. The effects of a sub-anaesthetic dose of ketamine on human selective attention. Neuropsychopharmacology. 2000;22(3):293–302.
Umbricht D, Schmid L, Koller R, Vollenweider FX, Hell D, Javitt DC. Ketamine-induced deficits in auditory and visual context-dependent processing in healthy volunteers. Arch Gen Psychiatry. 2000;57:1139–47.
Vann Jones S, Banerjee S, Smith AD, Refsum H, Lennox B. Elevated homocysteine and N-methyl-D-aspartate-receptor antibodies as a cause of behavioural and cognitive decline in 22q11. 2 deletion syndrome. Oxf Med Case Reports. 2017;2017(12):omx076.
Baker K, Baldeweg T, Sivagnanasundaram S, Scambler P, Skuse D. COMT Val108/158Met modifies mismatch negativity and cognitive function in 22q11 deletion syndrome. Biol Psychiat. 2005;58(1):23–31.
Cantonas LM, Mancini V, Rihs TA, Rochas V, Schneider M, Eliez S, et al. Abnormal auditory processing and underlying structural changes in 22q11. 2 deletion syndrome. Schizophrenia Bulletin. 2021;47(1):189–96.
Larsen KM, Mørup M, Birknow MR, Fischer E, Olsen L, Didriksen M, et al. Individuals with 22q11. 2 deletion syndrome show intact prediction but reduced adaptation in responses to repeated sounds: Evidence from Bayesian mapping. Neuroimage Clin. 2019;22:101721.
Zarchi O, Carmel M, Avni C, Attias J, Frisch A, Michaelovsky E, et al. Schizophrenia-like neurophysiological abnormalities in 22q11.2 deletion syndrome and their association to COMT and PRODH genotypes. J Psychiatr Res. 2013;47(11):1623–9.
Turetsky BI, Calkins ME, Light GA, Olincy A, Radant AD, Swerdlow NR. Neurophysiological endophenotypes of schizophrenia: the viability of selected candidate measures. Schizophr Bull. 2007;33(1):69–94.
Cattan S, Bachatene L, Bharmauria V, Jeyabalaratnam J, Milleret C, Molotchnikoff S. Comparative analysis of orientation maps in areas 17 and 18 of the cat primary visual cortex following adaptation. Eur J Neurosci. 2014;40(3):2554–63.
Muller JR, Metha AB, Krauskopf J, Lennie P. Rapid adaption in visual cortex to the structure of images. Science. 1999;285(5432):1405.
Wissig SC, Kohn A. The influence of surround suppression on adaptation effects in primary visual cortex. J Neurophysiol. 2012;107(12):3370–84.
Wark B, Lundstrom BN, Fairhall A. Sensory Adaptation. Curr Opin Neurobiol. 2007;17(4):423–9.
Roth WT, Krainz PL, Ford JM, Tinklenberg JR, Rothbart RM, Kopell BS. Parameters of temporal recovery of the human auditory evoked potential. Electroencephalogr Clin Neurophysiol. 1976;40(6):623–32.
Budd TW, Barry RJ, Gordon E, Rennie C, Michie PT. Decrement of the N1 auditory event-related potential with stimulus repetition: Habituation vs. refractoriness. Int J Psychophysiol. 1998;31:51–68.
Muller-Gass A, Marcoux A, Jamshidi P, Campbell K. The effects of very slow rates of stimulus presentation on event-related potential estimates of hearing threshold. Int J Audiol. 2008;47(1):34–43.
Pereira DR, Cardoso S, Ferreira-Santos F, Fernandes C, Cunha-Reis C, Paiva TO, et al. Effects of inter-stimulus interval (ISI) duration on the N1 and P2 components of the auditory event-related potential. Int J Psychophysiol. 2014;94(3):311–8.
Rosburg T, Zimmerer K, Huonker R. Short-term habituation of auditory evoked potential and neuromagnetic field components in dependence of the interstimulus interval. Exp Brain Res. 2010;205(4):559–70.
Larsson J, Smith AT. fMRI repetition suppression: neuronal adaptation or stimulus expectation? Cereb Cortex. 2012;22(3):567–76.
Chung S, Li X, Nelson SB. Short-term depression at thalamocortical synapses contributes to rapid adaptation of cortical sensory responses in vivo. Neuron. 2002;34(3):437–46.
Friston K. A theory of cortical responses Philosophical transactions of the Royal Society B. Biol Sci. 2005;360(1456):815–36.
Grill-Spector K, Henson R, Martin A. Repetition and the brain: neural models of stimulus-specific effects. Trends Cogn Sci. 2006;10(1):14–23.
Brockhaus-Dumke A, Mueller R, Faigle U, Klosterkoetter J. Sensory gating revisited: Relation between brain oscillations and auditory evoked potentials in schizophrenia. Schizophr Res. 2008;99(1–3):238–49.
Adler LE, Waldo MC, Freedman R. Neurophysiologic studies of sensory gating in schizophrenia: comparison of auditory and visual responses. Biol Psychiat. 1985;20(12):1284–96.
Braff DL, Light GA, Swerdlow NR. Prepulse inhibition and P50 suppression are both deficient but not correlated in schizophrenia patients. Biol Psychiat. 2007;61:1204–7.
Bramon E, Rabe-Hesketh S, Sham P, Murray RM, Frangou S. Meta-analysis of the P300 and P50 waveforms in schizophrenia. Schizophr Res. 2004;70:315–29.
Patterson JV, Hetrick WP, Boutros NN, Jin Y, Sandman C, Stern H, et al. P50 sensory gating ratios in schizophrenics and controls: a review and data analysis. Psychiatry Res. 2008;158(2):226–47.
Vorstman JA, Turetsky BI, Sijmens-Morcus ME, De Sain MG, Dorland B, Sprong M, et al. Proline affects brain function in 22q11DS children with the low activity COMT158 allele. Neuropsychopharmacology. 2009;34(3):739–46.
Freedman R, Adler LE, Myles-Worsley M, Nagamoto HT, Miller C, Kisley M, et al. Inhibitory gating of an evoked response to repeated auditory stimuli in schizophrenic and normal subjects: Human recordings, computer simulation, and an animal model. Arch Gen Psychiatry. 1996;53(12):1114–21.
Cowan N. An embedded-processes model of working memory. In: Miyake A, Shah P, editors. Models of Working Memory: Mechanisms of Active Maintenace and Executive Control. Cambridge, UK: Cambridge University Press; 1999. p. 62–101.
Näätänen R, Winkler I. The concept of auditory stimulus representation in cognitive neuroscience. Psychol Bull. 1999;125(6):826–59.
Näätänen R, Alho K. Mismatch negativity-A unique measure of sensory processing in audition. Int J Neurosci. 1995;80(1–4):317–37.
Ritter W, Deacon D, Gomes H, Javitt DC, Vaughan JH. The mismatch negativity of event-related potentials as a probe of transient auditory memory: A review. Ear Hear. 1995;16(1):52–67.
Ritter W, Sussman E, Molholm S, Foxe JJ. Memory reactivation or reinstatement and the mismatch negativity. Psychophysiology. 2002;39:158–65.
Javitt DC, Steinschneider M, Schroeder CE, Arezzo JC. Role of cortical N-methyl-D-aspartate receptors in auditory sensory memory and mismatch negativity generation: Implications for schizophrenia. Proc Natl Acad Sci. 1996;93(21):11962–7.
Erickson MA, Ruffle A, Gold JM. A meta-analysis of mismatch negativity in schizophrenia: From clinical risk to disease specificity and progression. Biol Psychiat. 2016;79(12):980–7.
Damaso KA, Michie PT, Todd J. Paying attention to MMN in schizophrenia. Brain Res. 2015;1626:267–79.
Bodatsch M, Ruhrmann S, Wagner M, Muller R, Schultze-Lutter F, Frommann I, et al. Prediction of psychosis by mismatch negativity. Biol Psychiat. 2011;69(10):959–66.
Jahshan C, Cadenhead KS, Rissling AJ, Kirihara K, Braff DL, Light GA. Automatic sensory information processing abnormalities across the illness course of schizophrenia. Psychol Med. 2011;42(1):85–97.
Atkinson RJ, Michie PT, Schall U. Duration mismatch negativity and P3a in first-episode psychosis and individuals at ultra-high risk of psychosis. Biol Psychiat. 2012;71(2):98–104.
Shaikh M, Valmaggia L, Broome MR, Dutt A, Lappin J, Day F, et al. Reduced mismatch negativity predates the onset of psychosis. Schizophr Res. 2012;134(1):42–8.
Shin KS, Kim JS, Kang DH, Koh Y, Choi JS, O’Donnell BF, et al. Pre-attentive auditory processing in ultra-high-risk for schizophrenia with magnetoencephalography. Biol Psychiat. 2009;65(12):1071–8.
Umbricht DS, Bates JA, Lieberman JA, Kane JM, Javitt DC. Electrophysiological indices of automatic and controlled auditory information processing in first-episode, recent-onset and chronic schizophrenia. Biol Psychiat. 2006;59(8):762–72.
Javitt DC, Shelley AM, Silipo G, Lieberman JA. Schizophrenia spectrum disorders in people with intellectual disabilities. Arch Gen Psychiatry. 2000;57:1131–7.
Todd J, Michie PT, Schall U, Karayanidis F, Yabe H, Naatanen R. Deviant matters: Duration, frequency, and intensity deviants reveal different patterns of mismatch negativity reduction in early and late schizophrenia. Biol Psychiat. 2008;63(1):58–64.
Michie PT, Budd TW, Todd J, Rock D, Wichmann H, Box J, et al. Duration and frequency mismatch negativity in schizophrenia. Clin Neurophysiol. 2000;111:1054–65.
Oades RD, Wild-Wall N, Juran SA, Sachsse J, Oknina LB, Ropcke B. Auditory change detection in schizophrenia: Sources of activity, related neuropsychological function and symptoms in patients with a first episode in adolescence, and patients 14 years after an adolescent illness-onset. BMC Psychiatry. 2006;6(7):1–14.
Brockhaus-Dumke A, Tendolkar I, Pukrop R, Schultze-Lutter F, Klosterkotter J, Ruhrmann S. Impaired mismatch negativity generation in prodromal subjects and patients with schizophrenia. Schizophr Res. 2005;73(2–3):297–310.
Oknina LB, Wild-Wall N, Oades RD, Juran SA, Ropcke B, Pfueller U, et al. Frontal and temporal sources of mismatch negativity in healthy controls, patients at onset of schizophrenia in adolescence and others at 15 years after onset. Schizophr Res. 2005;76(1):25–41.
Light GA, Braff DL. Mismatch negativity deficits are associated with poor functioning in schizophrenia patients. Arch Gen Psychiatry. 2005;62:127–36.
Magno E, Yeap S, Thakore JH, Garavan H, De Sanctis P, Foxe JJ. Are auditory-evoked frequency and duration mismatch negativity deficits endophenotypic for schizophrenia? High-density electrical mapping in clinically unaffected first-degree relatives and first-episode and chronic schizophrenia. Biol Psychiat. 2008;64(5):385–91.
Kiang M, Braff DL, Sprock J, Light GA. The relationship between preattentive sensory processing deficits and age in schizophrenia patients. Clin Neurophysiol. 2009;120(11):1949–57.
Leitman DI, Sehatpour P, Higgins BA, Foxe JJ, Silipo G, Javitt DC. Sensory deficits and distributed hierarchical dysfunction in schizophrenia. Am J Psychiatry. 2010;167(7):818–27.
Salisbury DF, Kohler J, Shenton ME, McCarley RW. Deficit Effect Sizes and Correlations of Auditory Event-Related Potentials at First Hospitalization in the Schizophrenia Spectrum. Clin EEG Neurosci. 2020;51(4):198–206.
Salisbury DF, McCathern AG, Coffman BA, Murphy TK, Haigh SM. Complex mismatch negativity to tone pair deviants in long-term schizophrenia and in the first-episode schizophrenia spectrum. Schizophr Res. 2018;191:18–24.
Salisbury DF, Polizzotto NR, Nestor PG, Haigh SM, Koehler J, McCarley RW. Pitch and Duration Mismatch Negativity and Premorbid Intellect in the First Hospitalized Schizophrenia Spectrum. Schizophr Bull. 2017;43(2):407–16.
Atkinson RJ, Fulham WR, Michie PT, Ward PB, Todd J, Stain H, et al. Electrophysiological, cognitive and clinical profiles of at-risk mental state: The longitudinal Minds in Transition (MinT) study. PLoS ONE. 2017;12(2):e0171657.
Larsen KM, Dzafic I, Siebner HR, Garrido MI. Alteration of functional brain architecture in 22q11.2 deletion syndrome-Insights into susceptibility for psychosis. NeuroImage. 2018:1–18.
Bartha-Doering L, Deuster D, Giordano V, am Zehnhoff-Dinnesen A, Dobel C. A systematic review of the mismatch negativity as an index for auditory sensory memory: From basic research to clinical and developmental perspectives. Psychophysiology. 2015;52:1115–30.
Andrade GN, Butler JS, Peters GA, Molholm S, Foxe JJ. Atypical visual and somatosensory adaptation in schizophrenia-spectrum disorders. Transl Psychiatry. 2016;6(5):1–14.
Dias EC, Butler PD, Hoptman MJ, Javitt DC. Early sensory contributions to contextual encoding deficits in schizophrenia. Arch Gen Psychiatry. 2011;68(7):654–64.
Butler PD, Martinez A, Foxe JJ, Kim D, Zemon V, Silipo G, et al. Subcortical visual dysfunction in schizophrenia drives secondary cortical impairments. Brain. 2007;130(2):417–30.
Pinheiro AP, Liu T, Nestor PG, McCarley RW, Gonçalves OF, Niznikiewicz MA. Visual emotional information processing in male schizophrenia patients: Combining ERP, clinical and behavioral evidence. Neurosci Lett. 2013;550:75–80.
Pokorny VJ, Lano TJ, Schallmo MP, Olman CA, Sponheim SR. Reduced influence of perceptual context in schizophrenia: Behavioral and neurophysiological evidence. Psychol Med. 2021;51(5):786–94.
Davenport ND, Sponheim SR, Stanwyck JJ. Neural anomalies during visual search in schizophrenia patients and unaffected siblings of schizophrenia patients. Schizophr Res. 2006;82(1):15–26.
Foxe JJ, Doniger GM, Javitt DC. Early visual processing deficits in schizophrenia: Impaired P1 generation revealed by high-density electrical mapping. NeuroReport. 2001;12(17):3815–20.
Yeap S, Kelly SP, Sehatpour P, Magno E, Garavan H, Thakore JH, et al. Visual sensory processing deficits in Schizophrenia and their relationship to disease state. Eur Arch Psychiatry Clin Neurosci. 2008;258(5):305–16.
Foxe JJ, Yeap S, Leavitt VM. Brief monocular deprivation as an assay of short-term visual sensory plasticity in schizophrenia–“the binocular effect.” Front Psych. 2013;4:164.
Yeap S, Kelly SP, Thakore JH, Foxe JJ. Visual sensory processing deficits in first-episode patients with Schizophrenia. Schizophr Res. 2008;1(102):340–3.
Lalor EC, Yeap S, Reilly RB, Pearlmutter BA, Foxe JJ. Dissecting the cellular contributions to early visual sensory processing deficits in schizophrenia using the VESPA evoked response. Schizophr Res. 2008;98(1–3):256–64.
Yeap S, Kelly SP, Sehatpour P, Magno E, Javitt DC, Garavan H, et al. Early visual sensory deficits as endophenotypes for schizophrenia: High-density electrical mapping in clinically unaffected first-degree relatives. Arch Gen Psychiatry. 2006;63(11):1180–8.
Biria M, Tomescu MI, Custo A, Cantonas LM, Song KW, Schneider M, et al. Visual processing deficits in 22q11. 2 deletion syndrome. NeuroImage: Clinical. 2018;17:976–86.
Magnée MJ, Lamme VA, de Sain-van der Velden MG, Vorstman JA, Kemner C. Proline and COMT status affect visual connectivity in children with 22q11. 2 deletion syndrome. PLoS One. 2011;6(10):e25882.
Van Amelsvoort T, Daly E, Robertson D, Suckling J, Ng V, Critchley H, et al. Structural brain abnormalities associated with deletion at chromosome 22q11: Quantitative neuroimaging study of adults with velo-cardio-facial syndrome. Br J Psychiatry. 2001;178(5):412–9.
Andrade GN, Butler JS, Mercier MR, Molholm S, Foxe JJ. Spatio-temporal dynamics of adaptation in the human visual system: A high-density electrical mapping study. Eur J Neurosci. 2015;41(7):925–39.
Schwertner A, Zortea M, Torres FV, Caumo W. Effects of subanesthetic ketamine administration on visual and auditory event-related potentials (ERP) in humans: A systematic review. Front Behav Neurosci. 2018;12:70.
Stephan KE, Friston KJ, Frith CD. Dysconnection in schizophrenia: from abnormal synaptic plasticity to failures of self-monitoring. Schizophr Bull. 2009;35(3):509–27.
Sesack SR, Carr DB, Omelchenko N, Pinto A. Anatomical substrates for glutamate-dopamine interactions: evidence for specificity of connections and extrasynaptic actions. Ann N Y Acad Sci. 2003;1003(1):36–52.
Zinkstok J, Schmitz N, Van Amelsvoort T, Moeton M, Baas F, Linszen D. Genetic variation in COMT and PRODH is associated with brain anatomy in patients with schizophrenia. Genes Brain Behav. 2008;7(1):61–9.
Wastell DG, Kleinman D. A psychoanatomical investigation of the locus of the mechanism responsible for the refractoriness of the visual vertex potential. Percept Psychophys. 1980;27(2):149–52.
Gjini K, Sundaresan K, Boutros NN. Electroencephalographic evidence of sensory gating in the occipital visual cortex. NeuroReport. 2008;19(15):1519–22.
Chen LC, Stropahl M, Schönwiesner M, Debener S. Enhanced visual adaptation in cochlear implant users revealed by concurrent EEG-fNIRS. Neuroimage. 2017;146:600–8.
Bradley C, Joyce N, Garcia-Larrea L. Adaptation in human somatosensory cortex as a model of sensory memory construction: A study using high-density EEG. Brain Struct Funct. 2016;221(1):421–31.
Jääskeläinen IP, Ahveninen J, Andermann ML, Belliveau JW, Raij T, Sams M. Short-term plasticity as a neural mechanism supporting memory and attentional functions. Brain Res. 2011;1422:66–81.
Simon TJ. A new account of the neurocognitive foundations of impairments in space, time, and number processing in children with chromosome 22q11. 2 deletion syndrome. Developmental Disabilities Res Reviews. 2008;14(1):52–8.
Shapiro HM, Takarae Y, Harvey DJ, Cabaral MH, Simon TJ. A cross-sectional study of the development of volitional control of spatial attention in children with chromosome 22q11. 2 deletion syndrome. J Neurodev Disord. 2012;4(1):1–12.
Campbell LE, Azuma R, Ambery F, Stevens A, Smith A, Morris RG. Executive functions and memory abilities in children with 22q11. 2 deletion syndrome. Aust N Z J Psychiatry. 2010;44(4):364–71.
Butler PD, Silverstein SM, Dakin SC. Visual perception and its impairment in schizophrenia. Biol Psychiat. 2008;64(1):40–7.
Kohn A. Visual adaptation: Physiology, mechanisms, and functional benefits. J Neurophysiol. 2007;97(5):3155–64.
Kapur S. Psychosis as a state of aberrant salience: A framework linking biology, phenomenology, and pharmacology in schizophrenia. Am J Psychiatry. 2003;160(1):13–23.
Gilbert SJ, Burgess PW. Executive function. Curr Biol. 2008;18(3):R110–4.
Aron AR. From reactive to proactive and selective control: Developing a richer model for stopping inappropriate responses. Biol Psychiat. 2011;69(12):55–68.
Fryer SL, Roach BJ, Ford JM, Donaldson KR, Calhoun VD, Pearlson GD, et al. Should I stay or should I go? FMRI study of response inhibition in early illness schizophrenia and risk for psychosis. Schizophr Bull. 2019;45(1):158–68.
Hester R, Foxe JJ, Molholm S, Shpaner M, Garavan H. Neural mechanisms involved in error processing: a comparison of errors made with and without awareness. Neuroimage. 2005;27(3):602–8.
Ford JM, Gray M, Whitfield SL, Turken U, Glover G, Faustman WO, et al. Acquiring and inhibiting prepotent responses in schizophrenia: event-related brain potentials and functional magnetic resonance imaging. Arch Gen Psychiatry. 2004;61(2):119–29.
Kiehl KA, Smith AM, Hare RD, Liddle PF. An event-related potential investigation of response inhibition in schizophrenia and psychopathy. Biol Psychiat. 2000;48(3):210–21.
Weisbrod M, Kiefer M, Marzinzik F, Spitzer M. Executive control is disturbed in schizophrenia: Evidence from event-related potentials in a Go/NoGo task. Biol Psychiat. 2000;47:51–60.
Hughes ME, Fulham WR, Johnston PJ, Michie PT. Stop-signal response inhibition in schizophrenia: Behavioural, event-related potential and functional neuroimaging data. Biol Psychol. 2012;89(1):220–31.
Chun J, Karam ZN, Marzinzik F, Kamali M, O’Donnell L, Tso IF, et al. Can P300 distinguish among schizophrenia, schizoaffective and bipolar I disorders? An ERP study of response inhibition. Schizophr Res. 2013;151(1–3):175–84.
Krakowski MI, De Sanctis P, Foxe JJ, Hoptman MJ, Nolan K, Kamiel S, et al. Disturbances in response inhibition and emotional processing as potential pathways to violence in schizophrenia: A high-density event-related potential study. Schizophr Bull. 2016;42(4):963–74.
Linton SR, Popa AM, Luck SJ, Bolden K, Carter CS, Niendam TA, et al. Neural and behavioral measures suggest that cognitive and affective functioning interactions mediate risk for psychosis‐proneness symptoms in youth with chromosome 22q11. 2 deletion syndrome. Am J Med Genet A. 2020;182(7):1615–30.
Romanos M, Ehlis AC, Baehne CG, Jacob C, Renner TJ, Storch A. Reduced NoGo-anteriorisation during continuous performance test in deletion syndrome 22q11. 2. J Psychiatr Res. 2010;44(12):768–74.
Eimer M. Effects of attention and stimulus probability on ERPs in a Go/No-Go task. Biol Psychol. 1993;35:123–38.
O’Connell RG, Dockree PM, Robertson IH, Bellgrove MA, Foxe JJ, Kelly SP. Uncovering the neural signature of lapsing attention: electrophysiological signals predict errors up to 20 s before they occur. J Neurosci. 2009;29(26):8604–11.
De Sanctis P, Butler JS, Malcolm BR, Foxe JJ. Recalibration of inhibitory control systems during walking-related dual-task interference: A mobile brain-body imaging (MOBI) study. Neuroimage. 2014;94:55–64.
Malcolm BR, Foxe JJ, Butler JS, De Sanctis P. The aging brain shows less flexible reallocation of cognitive resources during dual-task walking: A mobile brain/body imaging (MoBI) study. NeuroImage. 2015;117(230–242).
Morie KP, Garavan H, Bell RP, De Sanctis P, Krakowski MI, Foxe JJ. Intact inhibitory control processes in abstinent drug abusers (II): A high-density electrical mapping study in former cocaine and heroin addicts. Neuropharmacology. 2014;82:151–60.
Dockree PM, Kelly SP, Robertson IH, Reilly RB, Foxe JJ. Neurophysiological markers of alert responding during goal-directed behavior: A high-density electrical mapping study. Neuroimage. 2005;27(3):587–601.
Donkers FC, Van Boxtel GJ. The N2 in go/no-go tasks reflects conflict monitoring not response inhibition. Brain Cogn. 2004;56(2):165–76.
Groom MJ, Cragg L. Differential modulation of the N2 and P3 event-related potentials by response conflict and inhibition. Brain Cogn. 2015;97:1–9.
Bokura H, Yamaguchi S, Kobayashi S. Electrophysiological correlates for response inhibition in a Go/NoGo task. Clin Neurophysiol. 2001;112(12):2224–32.
Wessel JR, Aron AR. It’s not too late: The onset of the frontocentral P3 indexes successful response inhibition in the stop-signal paradigm. Psychophysiology. 2015;52(4):472–80.
Kiefer M, Marzinzik F, Weisbrod M, Scherg M, Spitzer M. The time course of brain activations during response inhibition: Evidence from event-related potentials in a go/no go task. NeuroReport. 1998;9(4):765–70.
Waller DA, Hazeltine E, Wessel JR. Common neural processes during action-stopping and infrequent stimulus detection: The frontocentral P3 as an index of generic motor inhibition. International Journal of Psychophysiology. 2019.
Bruin KJ, Wijers AA. Inhibition, response mode, and stimulus probability: a comparative event-related potential study. Clin Neurophysiol. 2002;113(7):1172–82.
Benvenuti SM, Sarlo M, Buodo G, Mento G, Palomba D. Influence of impulsiveness on emotional modulation of response inhibition: An ERP study. Clin Neurophysiol. 2015;126(10):1915–25.
Smith JL, Johnstone SJ, Barry RJ. Movement-related potentials in the Go/NoGo task: The P3 reflects both cognitive and motor inhibition. Clin Neurophysiol. 2008;119(3):704–14.
Wiersema JR, Roeyers H. ERP correlates of effortful control in children with varying levels of ADHD symptoms. J Abnorm Child Psychol. 2009;37(3):327–36.
Kropotov JD, Ponomarev VA, Hollup S, Mueller A. Dissociating action inhibition, conflict monitoring and sensory mismatch into independent components of event related potentials in GO/NOGO task. Neuroimage. 2011;57(2):565–75.
Randall WM, Smith JL. Conflict and inhibition in the cued-Go/NoGo task. Clin Neurophysiol. 2011;122(12):2400–7.
Smith JL, Johnstone SJ, Barry RJ. Response priming in the Go/NoGo task: The N2 reflects neither inhibition nor conflict. Clin Neurophysiol. 2007;118:343–55.
Groom MJ, Bates AT, Jackson GM, Calton TG, Liddle PF, Hollis C. Event-related potentials in adolescents with schizophrenia and their siblings: a comparison with attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder. Biol Psychiat. 2008;63(8):784–92.
Pfefferbaum A, Ford JM, White PM, Roth WT. P3 in schizophrenia is affected by stimulus modality, response requirements, medication status, and negative symptoms. Arch Gen Psychiatry. 1989;46(11):1035–44.
Kamarajan C, Porjesz B, Jones KA, Choi K, Chorlian DB, Padmanabhapillai A, et al. Alcoholism is a disinhibitory disorder: Neurophysiological evidence from a Go/No-Go task. Biol Psychol. 2005;69(3):353–73.
Fisher T, Aharon-Peretz J, Pratt H. Dis-regulation of response inhibition in adult Attention Deficit Hyperactivity Disorder (ADHD): An ERP study. Clin Neurophysiol. 2011;122(12):2390–9.
Nieuwenhuis S, Aston-Jones G, Cohen JD. Decision making, the P3, and the locus coeruleus–norepinephrine system. Psychol Bull. 2005;131(4):510.
Yamamoto K, Hornykiewicz O. Proposal for a noradrenaline hypothesis of schizophrenia. Prog Neuropsychopharmacol Biol Psychiatry. 2004;28(5):913–22.
Friedman JI, Adler DN, Davis KL. The role of norepinephrine in the pathophysiology of cognitive disorders: Potential applications to the treatment of cognitive dysfunction in schizophrenia and Alzheimer’s disease. Biol Psychiat. 1999;46:1243–52.
Taylor SF, Stern ER, Gehring WJ. Neural systems for error monitoring: recent findings and theoretical perspectives. Neuroscientist. 2007;13(2):160–72.
Buzzell GA, Richards JE, White LK, Barker TV, Pine DS, Fox NA. Development of the error-monitoring system from ages 9–35: Unique insight provided by MRI-constrained source localization of EEG. Neuroimage. 2017;157:13–26.
Falkenstein M, Hohnsbein J, Hoormann J, Blanke L. Effects of crossmodal divided attention on late ERP components. II. Error processing in choice reaction tasks. Electroencephalogr Clin Neurophysiol. 1991;78(6):447–55.
Nieuwenhuis S, Ridderinkhof KR, Blom J, Band GPH, Kok A. Error-related brain potentials are differentially related to awareness of response errors: Evidence from an antisaccade task. Psychophysiology. 2001;38:752–60.
Leuthold H, Sommer W. ERP correlates of error processing in spatial S-R compatibility tasks. Clin Neurophysiol. 1999;110:342–57.
O’Connell RG, Dockree PM, Bellgrove MA, Kelly SP, Hester R, Garavan H, et al. The role of cingulate cortex in the detection of errors with and without awareness: a high-density electrical mapping study. European Jornal of Neuroscience. 2007;25(8):2571–9.
Bates AT, Kiehl KA, Laurens KR, Liddle PF. Error-related negativity and correct response negativity in schizophrenia. Clin Neurophysiol. 2002;113(9):1454–63.
Kopp B, Rist F. An event-related brain potential substrate of disturbed response monitoring in paranoid schizophrenic patients. J Abnorm Psychol. 1999;108(2):337.
Mathalon DH, Fedor M, Faustman WO, Gray M, Askari N, Ford JM. Response-monitoring dysfunction in schizophrenia: An event-related brain potential study. J Abnorm Psychol. 2002;111(1):22.
Houthoofd S, Morrens M, Sabbe B, Schrijvers D, Vandendriessche F, Hulstijn W, et al. Trait and state aspects of internal and external performance monitoring in schizophrenia. Int J Psychophysiol. 2013;87(1):42–51.
Alain C, McNeely HE, He Y, Christensen BK, West R. Neurophysiological evidence of error-monitoring deficits in patients with schizophrenia. Cereb Cortex. 2002;12(8):840–6.
Laurens KR, Hodgins S, Mould GL, West SA, Schoenberg PL, Murray RM, et al. Error-related processing dysfunction in children aged 9 to 12 years presenting putative antecedents of schizophrenia. Biol Psychiat. 2010;67(3):238–45.
Perez VB, Ford JM, Roach BJ, Woods SW, McGlashan TH, Srihari VH, et al. Error monitoring dysfunction across the illness course of schizophrenia. J Abnorm Psychol. 2012;121(2):372.
Foti D, Kotov R, Bromet E, Hajcak G. Beyond the broken error-related negativity: Functional and diagnostic correlates of error processing in psychosis. Biol Psychiat. 2012;71(10):864–72.
Morris SE, Yee CM, Nuechterlein KH. Electrophysiological analysis of error monitoring in schizophrenia. J Abnorm Psychol. 2006;115(2):239.
Kim MS, Kang SS, Shin KS, Yoo SY, Kim YY, Kwon JS. Neuropsychological correlates of error negativity and positivity in schizophrenia patients. Psychiatry Clin Neurosci. 2006;60(3):303–11.
Khandaker GM, Barnett JH, White IR, Jones PB. A quantitative meta-analysis of population-based studies of premorbid intelligence and schizophrenia. Schizophr Res. 2011;132(2–3):220–7.
Acknowledgements
We wish to thank Dr. Juliana Bates, Alaina Berruti, Douwe Horsthuis, and Elise Taverna for their help with data collection. We would also like to acknowledge the role of the Montefiore-Einstein Regional Center for 22q11.2 deletion syndrome and the Rose F. Kennedy Intellectual and Developmental Disability Research Center for all their support. We extend our most sincere gratitude to the participants and their families for their interest, their involvement, and their time.
Funding
This work was supported in part by the Eunice Kennedy Shriver National Institute of Child Health and Human Development (NICHD), under award numbers U54 HD090260, P50 HD105352, and P50 HD10353.
Author information
Authors and Affiliations
Contributions
AAF wrote the first draft of the manuscript. SM and JJF provided editorial input on the subsequent drafts. All authors read and approved the final manuscript.
Corresponding authors
Ethics declarations
Ethics approval and consent to participate
All participants signed an informed consent and were monetarily compensated for their time. All studies reported and their procedures and materials received ethical approval by the Institutional Review Board of the Albert Einstein College of Medicine. All aspects of the research conformed to the tenets of the Declaration of Helsinki.
Consent for publication
Not applicable.
Competing interests
The authors declare that they have no competing interests.
Additional information
Publisher’s Note
Springer Nature remains neutral with regard to jurisdictional claims in published maps and institutional affiliations.
Rights and permissions
Open Access This article is licensed under a Creative Commons Attribution 4.0 International License, which permits use, sharing, adaptation, distribution and reproduction in any medium or format, as long as you give appropriate credit to the original author(s) and the source, provide a link to the Creative Commons licence, and indicate if changes were made. The images or other third party material in this article are included in the article's Creative Commons licence, unless indicated otherwise in a credit line to the material. If material is not included in the article's Creative Commons licence and your intended use is not permitted by statutory regulation or exceeds the permitted use, you will need to obtain permission directly from the copyright holder. To view a copy of this licence, visit http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/. The Creative Commons Public Domain Dedication waiver (http://creativecommons.org/publicdomain/zero/1.0/) applies to the data made available in this article, unless otherwise stated in a credit line to the data.
About this article
Cite this article
Francisco, A.A., Foxe, J.J. & Molholm, S. Event-related potential (ERP) markers of 22q11.2 deletion syndrome and associated psychosis. J Neurodevelop Disord 15, 19 (2023). https://doiorg.publicaciones.saludcastillayleon.es/10.1186/s11689-023-09487-9
Received:
Accepted:
Published:
DOI: https://doiorg.publicaciones.saludcastillayleon.es/10.1186/s11689-023-09487-9